Elsevier

Human Movement Science

Volume 32, Issue 1, February 2013, Pages 228-239
Human Movement Science

Precision control of trunk movement in low back pain patients

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2012.12.007Get rights and content

Abstract

Motor control is challenged in tasks with high precision demands. In such tasks, signal-dependent neuromuscular noise causes errors and proprioceptive feedback is required for optimal performance. Pain may affect proprioception, muscle activation patterns and resulting kinematics. Therefore, we investigated precision control of trunk movement in 18 low back pain (LBP) patients and 13 healthy control subjects. The subjects performed a spiral-tracking task requiring precise trunk movements, in conditions with and without disturbance of proprioception by lumbar muscle vibration. Tracking task performance and trunk muscle electromyography were recorded. In conditions without lumbar muscle vibration, tracking errors were 27.1% larger in LBP patients compared to healthy controls. Vibration caused tracking errors to increase by 10.5% in healthy controls, but not in LBP patients. These results suggest that reduced precision in LBP patients might be explained by proprioceptive deficits. Ratios of antagonistic over agonistic muscle activation were similar between groups. Tracking errors increased trunk inclination, but no significant relation between tracking error and agonistic muscle activation was found. Tracking errors did not decrease when antagonistic muscle activation increased, so, neither healthy subjects nor LBP patients appear to counteract trunk movement errors by increasing co-contraction.

Introduction

Precise motor control is hampered by neuromuscular noise. The exact origin of this noise is still unknown (Christakos et al., 2006, De Luca et al., 1982, Jones et al., 2002), but synaptic noise resulting in fluctuations in motor unit firing rates and firing intervals is suggested to be one of the main causes (Matthews, 1996, Selen et al., 2005). Neuromuscular noise is signal-dependent, in that force variability increases with muscle activation level (Allum et al., 1978, Christou et al., 2002, Jones et al., 2002, Newell and Carlton, 1988, Sherwood et al., 1988, Slifkin et al., 2000, Tseng et al., 2003, Visser et al., 2003). The effects of neuromuscular noise become apparent in a lack of precision, for example in aiming and tracking tasks. Performance on such tasks reflects the ability to reduce kinematic variability and can be used as a measure of quality of motor control.

Precise motor control appears to be impaired by pain. Huysmans and colleagues found larger upper limb tracking errors in subjects with shoulder pain compared to pain-free controls (Huysmans, Hoozemans, van der Beek, de Looze, & van Dieën, 2010). It has been suggested that this is due to the effect of nociceptive afference on muscle spindle feedback, which would impair proprioception (Pedersen, Sjolander, Wenngren, & Johansson, 1997). In addition, chronic pain has been shown to cause reorganization in the primary somatosensory cortex (Flor, Braun, Elbert, & Birbaumer, 1997), which may modulate the processing of both noxious and nonnoxious input (Moseley & Flor, 2012). In the study on shoulder pain the reduced precision indeed coincided with a reduced proprioceptive acuity in the pain group (Huysmans et al., 2010). Proprioceptive impairments in low back pain (LBP) patients have been demonstrated using lumbar muscle vibration, which is known to perturb proprioceptive feedback from muscle spindle afferents by inducing a lengthening illusion (Burke et al., 1976, Roll et al., 1989). Brumagne and colleagues found reduced trunk repositioning accuracy in LBP patients compared to healthy controls and, interestingly, paraspinal muscle vibration negatively affected trunk repositioning accuracy in healthy controls, but not in LBP patients (Brumagne, Cordo, Lysens, Verschueren, & Swinnen, 2000). Also, in a variety of postural tasks with vision occluded, the relative effects of lumbar muscle vibration and calf muscle vibration on postural sway differed between LBP patients and healthy controls (Brumagne et al., 2004, Brumagne et al., 2008, Claeys et al., 2011). LBP patients tended to use a more ankle-steered strategy, in that their response to calf muscle vibration was larger than their response to lumbar muscle vibration, which might point at a lower weighting of proprioceptive information from lumbar muscle spindles. Moreover, individuals with LBP have been shown to have increased levels of co-activation (van Dieën, Selen, & Cholewicki, 2003), which may indicate a compensatory joint stiffening strategy to deal with impaired proprioception.

Indeed, joint impedance modulation by antagonistic co-activation has been suggested as a means to counteract kinematic variability due to neuromuscular noise. Modeling work suggests that, by activating antagonistic muscle pairs around a joint, joint stiffness increases and kinematic variability decreases in spite of an increase in force variability of each of the muscles separately (Selen et al., 2005). In upper extremity tracking tasks, increased precision indeed coincided with increased joint impedance (Selen et al., 2006, Selen et al., 2006) and EMG activity (Huysmans et al., 2010), suggesting that the tracking error was successfully reduced by antagonistic co-activation.

Thus, whereas increased agonistic muscle activation can reduce precision, antagonistic co-activation can increase precision. In the trunk, however, no evidence for the use of such a co-activation strategy was found in static positioning tasks (Willigenburg, Kingma, & van Dieën, 2010). Instead, feedback control appeared to be used to regulate precision. Given this stronger reliance on feedback instead of co-activation to modulate precision in the trunk compared to the upper extremity and given the proprioceptive impairments associated with pain, we expect LBP to have pronounced effects on precision control of the trunk.

While tracking tasks are often called visuo-motor tasks, proprioceptive feedback is an important source of information in controlling tracking movements (Eidelberg and Davis, 1976, Nagaoka and Tanaka, 1981). In the present study, we therefore used a tracking task that required precise trunk movement with varying levels of trunk inclination, to investigate the effects of muscle activation level, lumbar muscle vibration and pain on kinematic error, as a measure of quality of trunk control. Subjects with and without LBP performed this tracking task with and without disturbance of proprioception through paraspinal muscle vibration. Muscle activity was continuously monitored by means of surface-electromyography (sEMG). We hypothesized that subjects with LBP would make larger tracking errors and that, as patients may rely less on proprioception, muscle vibration would have a larger effect in subjects without LBP. Furthermore, we hypothesized that tracking errors would increase with trunk inclination, and thus with the level of agonistic muscle activity. Ratios of antagonistic co-activation were hypothesized to be higher in the patient group.

Section snippets

Subjects

Eighteen subjects with non-specific LBP (31 ± 14 [M ± SD] years old, BMI 23.4 ± 2.4 kg/m2, 11 male) and 13 healthy controls (34 ± 12 years old, BMI 22.9 ± 2.4 kg/m2, 9 male) with no (history of) LBP participated in the experiment. No significant differences between the LBP and control groups were found in age and BMI and all subjects had normal or corrected to normal sight. One LBP patient was not able to participate due to a very limited range of lumbar motion. The protocol was approved by the local

Effects of pain, proprioception disturbance, and trunk inclination on tracking error

Results for GEE model 1 (Table 1), evaluating the effects of group (pain), vibration (proprioception disturbance), and trunk inclination revealed one significant interaction. In the absence of lumbar muscle vibration, the tracking error was significantly larger (27.1%) in LBP patients (0.422°) compared to healthy controls (0.332°, Fig. 5). In line with our hypothesis, vibration affected the performance in the control group more than in the LBP patients. Specifically, the tracking error

Discussion

The current study evaluated trunk motor control during a tracking task that required precise trunk movements with varying levels of trunk inclination. This task was performed by subjects with and without LBP, in conditions with and without lumbar muscle vibration. As hypothesized, tracking errors were higher in the LBP patients compared to healthy controls and increased with trunk inclination. Patients with more severe LBP made larger errors than patients with lower pain levels. In addition,

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Janneke de Bruin, Kim van Vijven, Nathalie Oomen and Sabrina Joosten for their assistance in subject recruitment and data collection.

References (36)

  • J.H. Allum et al.

    Neuronal mechanisms underlying physiological tremor

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1978)
  • S. Brumagne et al.

    The role of paraspinal muscle spindles in lumbosacral position sense in individuals with and without low back pain

    Spine

    (2000)
  • S. Brumagne et al.

    Persons with recurrent low back pain exhibit a rigid postural control strategy

    European Spine Journal

    (2008)
  • D. Burke et al.

    The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration during isometric contraction

    Journal of Physiology

    (1976)
  • C.N. Christakos et al.

    Parallel neuronal mechanisms underlying physiological force tremor in steady muscle contractions of humans

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (2006)
  • E.A. Christou et al.

    Modeling variability of force during isometric contractions of the quadriceps femoris

    Journal of Motor Behavior

    (2002)
  • K. Claeys et al.

    Decreased variability in postural control strategies in young people with non-specific low back pain is associated with altered proprioceptive reweighting

    European Journal of Applied Physiology

    (2011)
  • C.J. De Luca et al.

    Control scheme governing concurrently active human motor units during voluntary contractions

    Journal of Physiology

    (1982)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text